Skip to main content

Sex differences in the relationship between olfactory and cognitive impairment among subjects with subjective cognitive decline and mild cognitive impairment

Abstract

Background

Odor identification (OI) deficits are observed in both individuals with subjective cognitive decline (SCD) and mild cognitive impairment (MCI), and serve as risk factors for dementia. Compared with males, females typically demonstrate superior OI performance and different risks of dementia. However, the role of sex in the relationship between OI dysfunction and cognitive impairment remains uncertain.

Methods

In total, 121 subjects with SCD (41 males and 80 females), and 169 subjects with MCI (59 males and 110 females) underwent the Sniffin’ Sticks Screen 16 test and comprehensive neuropsychological examination. The relationships between olfactory and cognitive impairment were analyzed via partial correlation, multiple linear regression and moderating effects.

Results

In both SCD and MCI subjects, males performed better in language and females performed better in memory. The correlation between OI and cognition tended to be stronger in MCI subjects than in SCD subjects. In MCI subjects, the correlation tended to be stronger in females. For MCI females, better OI performance was correlated with higher short-term memory and attention scores. For MCI males, better OI performance was correlated with higher short-term memory scores. The OI was correlated with language in SCD males and with attention in SCD females. Sex played a moderating role in the relationship between OI dysfunction and language in MCI subjects and the relationship between OI dysfunction and short-term delayed recall memory and language in SCD subjects.

Conclusion

These findings revealed significant sex differences between OI dysfunction and cognitive impairment in SCD and MCI subjects. Sex differences should be considered when utilizing OI in clinical settings to predict cognitive function.

Plain language summary

Compared with males, females are at greater risk of dementia, and their olfactory abilities often demonstrate marked sex discrepancies that favor females. In this study, we investigated sex differences in olfaction and cognitive impairment and the role of sex in the relationship between olfactory and cognitive impairment. Because subjects with both subjective cognitive decline and mild cognitive impairment have olfactory impairment, we recruited a sample of 121 subjects with subjective cognitive decline (41 males and 80 females) and 169 subjects with mild cognitive impairment (59 males and 110 females) from the Affiliated Brain Hospital of Guangzhou Medical University and the community. We measured their olfaction and cognition. We found a positive correlation between olfaction and memory in mild cognitive impairment males, and olfaction was positively correlated with memory and attention in mild cognitive impairment females. Furthermore, sex served as a moderating factor in the relationship between olfaction and cognition among subjects with both subjective cognitive decline and mild cognitive impairment. Olfactory impairment may be helpful in predicting cognitive impairment in mild cognitive impairment subjects, and needs further follow-up, which is of great clinical importance for the use of olfaction to predict dementia in the future.

Highlights

  • Females tended to have a stronger relationship between olfaction and cognition in MCI subjects.

  • Olfactory performance was related to memory in MCI males, whereas it was related to memory and attention in MCI females.

  • Sex played a moderating role in the relationship between olfaction and cognition in both SCD and MCI subjects.

  • Our findings help elucidate the mechanisms of olfactory and cognitive impairment in males and females with SCD or MCI.

Background

As the older adult population continues to expand at a rapid pace worldwide, dementia has become a focal point of attention owing to its high occurrence in this demographic and the significant societal impact it has. Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the predominant type of dementia, potentially accounting for 60–70% of all cases [1]. There is still no cure for this disease, and early prediction remains essential. Diagnostic biomarkers include amyloid-β (Aβ) and tau [2], however, cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) [3] acquisition is an invasive process, and positron-emission tomography (PET) [4] is expensive and limited by the application of Aβ and tau tracers. Therefore, finding noninvasive, simple, and economical markers would facilitate mass adoption.

Ageing is often accompanied by a decline in odor capabilities [5, 6], but severe olfactory loss observed in patients with dementia [7]. Subjective cognitive decline (SCD) is the preclinical stage of dementia [8, 9], whereas mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is an early stage of dementia [10], and both have olfactory impairments [11, 12]. Olfactory capacity [13] is evaluated through tests that measure threshold, discrimination, identification, pleasantness, familiarity and other odors. According to several studies, the olfactory identification test alone can serve as a screening tool for olfactory dysfunction [14] and studies have proven that odor identification dysfunction can predict cognitive decline [11, 15, 16].

Sex differences are known to occur in the course of AD, as well as in olfaction. Studies have shown that more females have MCI and AD [17, 18]. Researchers using data from the French National Alzheimer Database (BNA) [19] reported that females presented a 2–3% greater likelihood of transitioning from MCI to AD. Numerous studies have explored sex differences in olfactory ability. The National Geographic Smell Survey (NGSS) [20] involving 1.2 million people revealed that females were able to correctly identify more odors. The Olfaction in Catalonia (LOFACAT) [21] survey investigated olfaction in the general population, based on a study of 9348 surveys of both sexes and all ages, researchers revealed that olfaction was better in females than in males. However, the sex-related associations between olfaction and cognition in subjects with SCD and MCI remain uncertain.

Therefore, the aims of the current study were to explore (1) sex differences in olfaction and cognition in subjects with MCI and SCD; (2) sex differences in the correlation of olfaction and cognition in MCI and SCD subjects; and (3) whether sex moderates the relationship between olfaction and cognition in MCI and SCD subjects. This study provides a deeper understanding of the effects of sex on olfaction and cognition in MCI and SCD subjects to guide the use of olfaction to predict cognition.

Methods

Subjects

A total of 290 subjects (169 with MCI, 121 with SCD) were continuously recruited from the Affiliated Brain Hospital of Guangzhou Medical University and the community in Guangzhou. All subjects or their legal guardians provided signed informed consent to participate in the study. The ethics committees of the Affiliated Brain Hospital of Guangzhou Medical University approved this study.

The diagnostic criteria for MCI were based on the Peterson criteria [22]. The SCD criteria include two major features [23]: a self-experienced persistent decline in cognitive capacity relative to a previously normal cognitive status unrelated to an acute event, and after adjusting for age, sex and years of education, compared with normal controls, the score difference of each test in the neuropsychological battery was less than 1.5 standard deviations. All the subjects underwent structured interviews, standardized olfactory tests, and clinical symptom and comprehensive cognitive assessments.

Assessment of cognitive function

All the subjects were interviewed by neuropsychologists to assess global cognitive function using Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE, 30 points, > 24 as normal) [24], five cognitive domains including memory (Auditory Verbal Learning Task, AVLT) [25], language (Boston Naming Test, BNT [26], 30 points, > 22 as normal and Verbal Fluency Test, VFT [27], > 10 as normal), executive function (Stroop Color and Word Test C, Stroop C [28], with scores based on how many seconds were required to complete the exam), visuospatial skill (Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, ROCF [29], 36 points, > 30 as normal) and attention (Symbol-Digit Modality Test, SDMT [30], > 28 as normal, Digit Span Test, DST [31], 24 points, > 19 as normal and Trail-Making Test A, TMT A [32], with scores based on how many seconds were required to complete the exam). The AVLT N1-3 (sum scores of AVLT N1, N2, and N3), AVLT N4, AVLT N5, and AVLT N6 represent immediate recall, short-term delayed recall, long-term delayed recall and recognition, respectively (36 points, 12 points, 12 points, and 24 points, respectively).

Assessment of olfactory function

For OI assessment, the Sniffin’ Sticks Screen 16 test [13] was applied, which involves the presentation of odorants from felt-tip pens. To measure OI performance, odorized pens were used. The pen’s cap was opened by the experimenter for approximately 3 s, and the pen’s tip was placed approximately 2 cm in front of both nostrils. The subjects were asked to smell 16 common odorants from the felt-tip pens and to name the odors using a multiple-choice format with 4 choices, only 1 of which was correct. The subjects’ scores ranged from 0 to 16. An OI score less than 10 was defined as OI dysfunction [33].

Data analysis

Demographic and clinical data analysis and data visualization were done by using SPSS 25.0 (SPSS, Chicago, IL, USA), R version 4.3.2 (R Core Team, R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria) and RStudio 2023.09.1 (RStudio Team. RStudio Inc., Boston, MA, USA). The differences in demographic, clinical, cognitive and olfactory information between males and females were analyzed using T-test for two independent samples, Mann–Whitney U test or χ2 test. Partial correlations were used to investigate the correlation between the cognitive scores and the values of OI, controlling variables included age, years of education. To investigate whether the factors influencing OI differed between males and females, we used multiple linear regression analysis, with the variables from the partial correlation analysis. The Hayes PROCESS macro was used to assess the moderating effect of sex on the relationship between OI dysfunction and cognitive function. OI was the predictor variable, cognition was the outcome variable, age and years of education were the covariates.

Results

Demographic, clinical, cognitive and olfactory information

In this study, a total of 290 people, 169 with MCI (59 males and 110 females), and 121 with SCD (41 males and 80 females) were included. As shown in Table 1, males had significantly higher BNT scores (p < 0.05) than females for both MCI and SCD subjects. In the population, females were more likely to have higher AVLT scores, including N1-3, N4, N5, and N6 scores (p < 0.05) in MCI females, and with the exception of AVLT N6 scores, the remaining AVLT scores were also higher in SCD females. No sex differences in demographic, clinical or olfactory information were observed.

Table 1 Demographic data, clinical information, cognitive function, and olfactory function of male and female MCI and SCD subjects

Partial correlation analyses

As shown in Table S1, after controlling for age and years of education, the OI score was found to be related to the MMSE, AVLT N1-3, AVLT N6, BNT, VFT and SDMT in males with MCI. Body mass index (BMI), neuropsychiatric inventory (NPI), MMSE, AVLT N1-3, AVLT N4, AVLT N5, BNT, Stroop C, TMT A, ROCF, SDMT and DST were correlated with OI score in MCI females. In SCD subjects, the OI score was associated with VFT in males, and SDMT in females. The correlations between males and females are shown in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Partial correlation between OI and other variables (control variables included age, years of education). A OI was associated with MMSE in MCI females (r = 0.299, p = 0.002) and MCI males (r = 0.294, p = 0.029). B OI was associated with AVLT N1-3 in MCI females (r = 0.349, p < 0.001) and MCI males (r = 0.353, p = 0.008). C OI was associated with VFT in MCI males (r = 0.372, p = 0.028) but not in MCI females. D OI was associated with DST in MCI females (r = 0.258, p = 0.007) but not in MCI males. E OI was not associated with AVLT N4 in SCD males or females. F OI was not associated with BNT in SCD males or females. MCI, mild cognitive impairment; SCD, subjective cognitive decline; OI, odor identification; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; AVLT N1-3, Auditory Verbal Learning Test Immediate recall; AVLT N4, Auditory Verbal Learning Test Short-term delayed recall; VFT, Verbal Fluency Test; DST, Digit Span Test; BNT, Boston Naming Test

Multiple linear regression

For males with MCI, the model p = 0.006 indicated that this regression equation was valid. The model adjusted R2 was 0.164, indicating that MMSE, AVLT N1-3, AVLT N6, BNT, VFT, and SDMT could explain 16.4% of the change in OI score. AVLT N1-3 (B = 0.279, p = 0.006) had a positive effect on OI score, whereas the remaining variables had no significant effect. For females with MCI, the regression analysis between clinical information, cognition and the OI score revealed a significant relationship between the OI score and AVLT N1-3 (B = 0.143, p < 0.001), and DST (B = 0.192, p = 0.041), with an adjusted R2 of 0.177 (see Table 2).

Table 2 Results of the stepwise linear regression between OI score and cognitive impairment in MCI subjects

Moderating analyses

For the moderation analysis between OI dysfunction and cognitive function with age and years of education as covariates, the overall model (R2 = 0.099, p = 0.044) and the moderating effect of sex (β = − 2.060, p = 0.029) on the relationship between OI dysfunction and VFT were significant in MCI subjects (Table 3, Fig. 2A). In SCD subjects, the moderating effect of sex on the relationship between OI dysfunction and AVLT N4 (β = 2.120, p = 0.021, overall model R2 = 0.196, p < 0.001) was significant. Additionally, both the overall model (R2 = 0.207, p < 0.001) and the moderating effect of sex on the relationship between OI dysfunction and BNT (β = 2.589, p = 0.023) were significant (Table 4, Fig. 2B, C).

Fig. 2
figure 2

A Conceptual and statistical model of the association between OI dysfunction and VFT moderated by sex in MCI subjects. B Conceptual and statistical model of the association between OI dysfunction and AVLT N4 moderated by sex in SCD subjects. C Conceptual and statistical model of the association between OI dysfunction and BNT moderated by sex in SCD subjects

Table 3 Process moderating effect model in MCI subjects
Table 4 Process moderating effect model in SCD subjects

Discussion

To the best of our knowledges, the present work is the first to compare sex differences in the relationships between OI and cognition in Chinese individuals with SCD and MCI. The key discoveries were as follows. (1) The correlation between OI and cognition tends to be stronger in MCI subjects than in SCD subjects. For SCD subjects, OI was associated with language in males and with attention in females. (2) In MCI subjects, the correlation between olfaction and cognition tends to be stronger in females than in males. The OI was associated with global cognition, memory, language, and attention in males with MCI. In females with MCI, OI was associated with global cognition, memory, language, executive function, visuospatial skills, attention, BMI, and NPI. (3) A greater number of factors influencing olfaction were present in MCI females. The primary factor influencing olfaction in males with MCI was memory, whereas in females with MCI, it was memory and attention. (4) Sex moderated the association between OI dysfunction and language in subjects with MCI, and moderated the relationship between OI dysfunction and both memory and language in SCD subjects.

This study revealed sex differences in some tasks, with males performing better on BNT and females performing better on AVLT in the two groups. There was no difference in the other tasks. As expected, the finding on the AVLT is in line with previous studies showing better female performance in memory tasks [34, 35]. Females are usually thought to excel in verbal ability but we found that females did not have a significant advantage in VFT. The finding on verbal fluency is consistent with a previous study [36] that demonstrated similar performance in verbal fluency between both sexes among healthy older controls and MCI subjects (p > 0.05), although the sample sizes in the present study were generally larger (59 males and 110 females with MCI vs. 28 males and 15 females; 41 males and 80 females with SCD vs. 28 males and 23 females in the healthy control group). Additionally, males in this study exhibited significantly better performance in the language domain for BNT than females did, which is in line with the findings of some studies. In numerous normative studies assessing BNT, sex is associated with naming performance, with males outperforming females [37,38,39].

Zhong et al. [40] conducted a study on OI and cognition using 18 questions from the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) in older Americans living in the community. They found that the association between OI and cognition was more pronounced in female with MCI than in males. Our study confirmed this finding, showing a stronger relationship between OI and cognition in females with MCI compared to males. However, the present study was conducted in Chinese individuals with SCD and MCI, and cognitive domains were measured via a number of representative scales. There is growing evidence that deficits in OI are associated with cognitive impairment in older adults [41, 42]. The association between OI and cognition was less pronounced in subjects with SCD than in those with MCI. One possibility to explain this finding may be that in the mild cognitive impairment stage, there is already an objective cognitive decline, whereas in the subjective cognitive decline stage, the subjects may only have subjective cognitive problems, so the relationship between olfaction and cognition was not as intimate in SCD subjects.

For MCI males, OI scores and memory scores were positively correlated, and for MCI females, higher memory and attention scores were related to higher OI scores. Many studies have reported that OI is significantly associated with memory [42,43,44,45]. However, our finding that OI performance predicts impairments in memory and attention in MCI females is novel. Olfaction and attention are closely related; the more concentrated the attention is, the stronger the sensitivity of olfaction [46], and olfactory information can reflexively guide visual attention [47]. Females among MCI subjects had higher mean OI scores, although the difference was not statistically significant, which might have led to the association between OI and attention in MCI females. Similarly, in SCD females, OI was associated with attention. Wang et al. [16] found that OI dysfunction worsens with increasing severity in the AD disease spectrum. Compared with MCI females, SCD females have less impairment of olfaction, which might explain why females with SCD also exhibited this association. In SCD males, OI was associated with language. Holly et al. [48] reported that language ability is a significant predictor of olfactory performance. In our study, males with SCD outperformed females in language tasks, which might partially account for the observed association in these males. Additionally, sex differences in brain connectivity and network organization, as well as the influence of sex hormones, could affect this relationship. Variations in brain networks between sexes may alter the processing of olfactory information, leading to associations between olfactory performance and different cognitive functions [49, 50]. The effects of sex hormones such as estrogen and testosterone on cognitive function and sensory processing have been extensively studied. Estrogen replacement therapy is believed to increase attention in females [51], whereas testosterone may inhibit the practice effect in verbal fluency tasks [52].

This study further investigated the impact of sex on the association between OI dysfunction and cognition in older Chinese individuals with SCD and MCI. Our findings showed that sex moderated the relationship between OI dysfunction and language in both groups; additionally, in SCD subjects, sex moderated the relationship between OI dysfunction and memory. Although our study did not reveal a difference in OI between males and females, there were sex differences in the relationship between OI and cognition. Sex plays an important role in language. Females are often considered to have an advantage in language skills, typically excelling in both comprehension and expression [53]. In semantic decision tasks, females tend to perform faster than males do [54]. However, some studies suggest that males may have advantages in specific language tasks, such as naming tests [37,38,39], and they show greater left frontal activation during semantic retrieval [55]. In language processing, males exhibit more inhibitory connections from the inferior frontal gyrus to the superior temporal gyrus (STG), whereas females show more inhibitory connections from the superior parietal lobule to the STG [54]. These differences may explain why sex moderates the relationship between olfactory performance and language. There are differences between sexes in the memory domain, with females having a slight advantage over males. During episodic memory tasks, females show more structural covariance connections than males do [56]. The brain activation patterns of males and females are notably different. When performing working memory tasks, females are predominantly activated in the left hemisphere, whereas males exhibit bilateral or right-sided activation patterns [57]. These differences may explain why sex plays a moderating role in olfactory impairment and memory.

There are several limitations in the present study. First, because the study was cross-sectional, it was unable to actively track changes in olfactory and cognitive functions, which need to be refined after the research. Second, the current evaluations did not consider olfactory thresholds or olfactory discrimination, and future research should undertake a thorough evaluation of olfactory function, encompassing olfactory identification, olfactory thresholds, and olfactory discrimination. Third, imaging, pathological and other markers are lacking, so our sample is based on the entire population, which may lead to a limited representation of subjects with MCI and SCD, and further imaging and biomarker data must be collected.

Perspectives and significance

The current study demonstrated the importance of considering sex differences in the relationship between olfaction and cognition in subjects with SCD and MCI. These findings contribute to a deeper understanding of how sex influences the interplay between olfaction and cognition in older adult populations, and they also aid in understanding the mechanisms of olfactory and cognitive impairment across sexes.

Availability of data and materials

No datasets were generated or analysed during the current study.

References

  1. Dementia. Accessed March 10, 2024. https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/dementia.

  2. Bloom GS. Amyloid-β and tau: the trigger and bullet in Alzheimer disease pathogenesis. JAMA Neurol. 2014;71(4):505–8. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1001/jamaneurol.2013.5847.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Lista S, Zetterberg H, Dubois B, Blennow K, Hampel H. Cerebrospinal fluid analysis in Alzheimer’s disease: technical issues and future developments. J Neurol. 2014;261(6):1234–43. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s00415-014-7366-z.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Platt B, Welch A, Riedel G. FDG-PET imaging, EEG and sleep phenotypes as translational biomarkers for research in Alzheimer’s disease. Biochem Soc Trans. 2011;39(4):874–80. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1042/BST0390874.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Attems J, Walker L, Jellinger KA. Olfaction and aging: a mini-review. Gerontology. 2015;61(6):485–90. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1159/000381619.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Doty RL, Shaman P, Applebaum SL, Giberson R, Siksorski L, Rosenberg L. Smell identification ability: changes with age. Science. 1984;226(4681):1441–3. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1126/science.6505700.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Mesholam RI, Moberg PJ, Mahr RN, Doty RL. Olfaction in neurodegenerative disease: a meta-analysis of olfactory functioning in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases. Arch Neurol. 1998;55(1):84–90. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1001/archneur.55.1.84.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Subjective Cognitive Decline in Older Adults: An Overview of Self-Report Measures Used Across 19 International Research Studies—PubMed. Accessed March 10, 2024. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/26402085/.

  9. Rabin LA, Smart CM, Amariglio RE. Subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2017;13:369–96. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032816-045136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Albert MS, DeKosky ST, Dickson D, et al. The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement J Alzheimers Assoc. 2011;7(3):270–9. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Thomas AJ, Hamilton CA, Barker S, et al. Olfactory impairment in mild cognitive impairment with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s disease. Int Psychogeriatr. 2022;34(6):585–92. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1017/S1041610221001265.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Jobin B, Zahal R, Bussières EL, Frasnelli J, Boller B. Olfactory identification in subjective cognitive decline: a meta-analysis. J Alzheimers Dis. 2021;79(4):1497–507. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3233/JAD-201022.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Hummel T, Sekinger B, Wolf SR, Pauli E, Kobal G. “Sniffin” sticks’: olfactory performance assessed by the combined testing of odor identification, odor discrimination and olfactory threshold. Chem Sens. 1997;22(1):39–52. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1093/chemse/22.1.39.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Tahmasebi R, Zehetmayer S, Pusswald G, Kovacs G, Stögmann E, Lehrner J. Identification of odors, faces, cities and naming of objects in patients with subjective cognitive decline, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: a longitudinal study. Int Psychogeriatr. 2019;31(04):537–49. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1017/S1041610218001114.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Schubert CR, Carmichael LL, Murphy C, Klein BEK, Klein R, Cruickshanks KJ. Olfaction and the 5-year incidence of cognitive impairment in an epidemiological study of older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2008;56(8):1517–21. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01826.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Wang Q, Chen B, Zhong X, et al. Olfactory dysfunction is already present with subjective cognitive decline and deepens with disease severity in the Alzheimer’s disease spectrum. J Alzheimers Dis. 2021;79(2):585–95. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3233/JAD-201168.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Mielke MM, Vemuri P, Rocca WA. Clinical epidemiology of Alzheimer’s disease: assessing sex and gender differences. Clin Epidemiol. 2014;6:37–48. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.2147/CLEP.S37929.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. 2023 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimers Dement. 2023;19(4):1598–1695. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1002/alz.13016.

  19. Pradier C, Sakarovitch C, Le Duff F, et al. The mini mental state examination at the time of Alzheimer’s disease and related disorders diagnosis, according to age, education, gender and place of residence: a cross-sectional study among the French National Alzheimer database. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(8): e103630. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1371/journal.pone.0103630.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Wysocki CJ, Gilbert AN. National geographic smell survey: effects of age are heterogenous. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1989;561(1):12–28. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1989.tb20966.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Mullol J, Alobid I, Mariño-Sánchez F, et al. Furthering the understanding of olfaction, prevalence of loss of smell and risk factors: a population-based survey (OLFACAT study). BMJ Open. 2012;2(6): e001256. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1136/bmjopen-2012-001256.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med. 2004;256(3):183–94. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Jessen F, Amariglio RE, van Boxtel M, et al. A conceptual framework for research on subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement J Alzheimers Assoc. 2014;10(6):844–52. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.jalz.2014.01.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician—PubMed. Accessed March 11, 2024. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/1202204/.

  25. Zhao Q, Lv Y, Zhou Y, Hong Z, Guo Q. Short-term delayed recall of auditory verbal learning test is equivalent to long-term delayed recall for identifying amnestic mild cognitive impairment. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(12): e51157. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1371/journal.pone.0051157.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Culture Effects on the Chinese Version Boston Naming Test Performance and the Normative Data in the Native Chinese-Speaking Elders in Mainland China—PubMed. Accessed March 11, 2024. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/35645954/.

  27. Song M, Suda M, Aoyama Y, et al. Similar activation patterns in the prefrontal cortex for Chinese and Japanese verbal fluency tests with syllable cues as revealed by near-infrared spectroscopy. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2020;42(9):924–31. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/13803395.2020.1825637.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Scarpina F, Tagini S. The stroop color and word test. Front Psychol. 2017;8:557. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00557.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Application of Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test in Chinese Normal Old People | Semantic Scholar. Accessed March 11, 2024. https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/Application-of-Rey-Osterrieth-Complex-Figure-Test-Zhen/eecc9cf1e46eb00ce40dd0a87ee7a1d48674504e.

  30. Sheridan LK, Fitzgerald HE, Adams KM, et al. Normative Symbol Digit Modalities Test performance in a community-based sample. Arch Clin Neuropsychol Off J Natl Acad Neuropsychol. 2006;21(1):23–8. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.acn.2005.07.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Kessels RPC, van den Berg E, Ruis C, Brands AMA. The backward span of the Corsi Block-Tapping Task and its association with the WAIS-III Digit Span. Assessment. 2008;15(4):426–34. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1177/1073191108315611.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Guo Y. A selective review of the ability for variants of the Trail Making Test to assess cognitive impairment. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2022;29(6):1634–45. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/23279095.2021.1887870.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Oleszkiewicz A, Schriever VA, Croy I, Hähner A, Hummel T. Updated Sniffin’ Sticks normative data based on an extended sample of 9139 subjects. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol. 2019;276(3):719–28. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s00405-018-5248-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Lee LK, Shahar S, Chin AV, Mohd Yusoff NA, Rajab N, Aziz SA. Prevalence of gender disparities and predictors affecting the occurrence of mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2012;54(1):185–91. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.archger.2011.03.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Andreano JM, Cahill L. Sex influences on the neurobiology of learning and memory. Learn Mem Cold Spring Harb N. 2009;16(4):248–66. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1101/lm.918309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Beinhoff U, Tumani H, Brettschneider J, Bittner D, Riepe MW. Gender-specificities in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. J Neurol. 2008;255(1):117–22. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s00415-008-0726-9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Bezdicek O, Rosická AM, Mana J, Libon DJ, Kopeček M, Georgi H. The 30-item and 15-item Boston naming test Czech version: Item response analysis and normative values for healthy older adults. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2021;43(9):890–905. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/13803395.2022.2029360.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Jefferson AL, Wong S, Gracer TS, Ozonoff A, Green RC, Stern RA. Geriatric performance on an abbreviated version of the Boston naming test. Appl Neuropsychol. 2007;14(3):215–23. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/09084280701509166.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Fällman K, Wressle E, Marcusson J, Classon E. Swedish normative data and longitudinal effects of aging for older adults: the Boston Naming Test 30-item and a short version of the Token Test. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2022. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/23279095.2022.2148106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Zhong S, Wroblewski KE, Laumann EO, McClintock MK, Pinto JM. Assessing how age, sex, race, and education affect the relationships between cognitive domains and odor identification. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2023;37(2):128–33. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1097/WAD.0000000000000554.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Devanand DP. Olfactory identification deficits, cognitive decline, and dementia in older adults. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2016;24(12):1151–7. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.jagp.2016.08.010.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Wang MC, Chiou JM, Chen YC, Chen JH. Association between olfactory dysfunction and cognitive impairment in dementia-free older adults: a prospective cohort study in Taiwan. J Alzheimers Dis. 2023;96(4):1477–88. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3233/JAD-230319.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Delgado-Lima AH, Bouhaben J, Martínez-Zujeros S, Pallardo-Rodil B, Gómez-Pavón J, Delgado-Losada ML. Could olfactory identification be a prognostic factor in detecting cognitive impairment risk in the elderly? GeroScience. 2023;45(3):2011–25. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s11357-023-00779-5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Joint trajectories of episodic memory and odor identification in older adults: patterns and predictors—PubMed. Accessed April 13, 2024. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/34232918/.

  45. Demographic and cognitive predictors of cued odor identification: evidence from a population-based study—PubMed. Accessed April 13, 2024. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15269128/.

  46. Attention to odor modulates thalamocortical connectivity in the human brain—PubMed. Accessed April 13, 2024. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/18480282/.

  47. Chen K, Zhou B, Chen S, He S, Zhou W. Olfaction spontaneously highlights visual saliency map. Proc Biol Sci. 2013;280(1768):20131729. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1098/rspb.2013.1729.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Westervelt H, Ruffolo J, Tremont G. Assessing olfaction in the neuropsychological exam: the relationship between odor identification and cognition in older adults. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2005;20(6):761–9. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.acn.2005.04.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Yang CC, Totzek JF, Lepage M, Lavigne KM. Sex differences in cognition and structural covariance-based morphometric connectivity: evidence from 28,000+ UK Biobank participants. Cereb Cortex N Y N 1991. 2023;33(19):10341–54. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1093/cercor/bhad286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Tomasi D, Volkow ND. Gender differences in brain functional connectivity density. Hum Brain Mapp. 2012;33(4):849–60. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1002/hbm.21252.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Hormone Replacement Therapy: What to Know. Verywell Health. Accessed October 29, 2024. https://www.verywellhealth.com/hormone-replacement-therapy-5271199.

  52. Wolf OT, Preut R, Hellhammer DH, Kudielka BM, Schürmeyer TH, Kirschbaum C. Testosterone and cognition in elderly men: a single testosterone injection blocks the practice effect in verbal fluency, but has no effect on spatial or verbal memory. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;47(7):650–4. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00145-6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. The Speech and Language of Children Aged 25 Months: Descriptive Data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children | Semantic Scholar. Accessed November 5, 2024. https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/The-Speech-and-Language-of-Children-Aged-25-Months%3A-Roulstone-Loader/ea820388fbac41e20a1b8859156a0376e2e74164.

  54. Xu M, Liang X, Ou J, Li H, Luo Y, Tan LH. Sex differences in functional brain networks for language. Cereb Cortex. 2020;30(3):1528–37. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1093/cercor/bhz184.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Konrad C, Engelien A, Schöning S, et al. The functional anatomy of semantic retrieval is influenced by gender, menstrual cycle, and sex hormones. J Neural Transm Vienna Austria 1996. 2008;115(9):1327–37. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s00702-008-0073-0.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  56. Shi Y, Cui D, Niu J, et al. Sex differences in structural covariance network based on MRI cortical morphometry: effects on episodic memory. Cereb Cortex. 2023;33(13):8645–53. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1093/cercor/bhad147.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Speck O, Ernst T, Braun J, Koch C, Miller E, Chang L. Gender differences in the functional organization of the brain for working memory. NeuroReport. 2000;11(11):2581.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank all the researchers and participants involved in this study.

Funding

This work was supported by Guangdong Province Key Areas Research and Development Programs-Brain Science and Brain-Inspired Intelligence Technology, Industry-University-Research Innovation Fund for Chinese Universities (2023HT022), Guangzhou Key Research and Development Program (2023B03J1296), Guangzhou Traditional Chinese Medicine and Integrated Traditional Chinese and Western Medicine Science and Technology Project (20222A011013), Basic and Applied Basic Research Project of Guangzhou Basic Research Program (202201010491) and Guangzhou Research-oriented Hospital. The funding source had no role in the study design, analysis, or interpretation of data or in the preparation of the report or decision to publish. The views expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the position or policy of the funders.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Qin Liu and Ben Chen contributed equally to this work. QL conducted the study, collected and analyzed the data, and drafted the first manuscript. BC conceptualized the study, interpreted the data and revised the study, QW, DX, MY, and YZ collected the data and revised the study, GL, JL, SL, KY and JL assisted in data collections. All authors red and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Xiaomei Zhong or Yuping Ning.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The ethics committees of the Affiliated Brain Hospital of Guangzhou Medical University approved the study procedures. All participants signed an informed consent prior to the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Liu, Q., Chen, B., Wang, Q. et al. Sex differences in the relationship between olfactory and cognitive impairment among subjects with subjective cognitive decline and mild cognitive impairment. Biol Sex Differ 16, 12 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s13293-025-00691-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s13293-025-00691-x

Keywords